EDITED BY
Eva Unternaehrer,
University Psychiatric Clinic Basel, Switzerland
REVIEWED BY
Roma Jusiene,
Vilnius University, Lithuania
Katherine Cost,
McMaster University, Canada
*
CORRESPONDENCE
V. Konok
RECEIVED 11 August 2023
ACCEPTED 13 May 2024
PUBLISHED 28 June 2024
CITATION
Konok V, Binet M-A, Korom Á, Pogány Á,
Miklósi Á and Fitzpatrick C (2024) Cure for
tantrums? Longitudinal associations between
parental digital emotion regulation and
childrens self-regulatory skills.
Front. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 3:1276154.
doi: 10.3389/frcha.2024.1276154
COPYRIGHT
© 2024 Konok, Binet, Korom, Pogány, Miklósi
and Fitzpatrick. This is an open-access article
distributed under the terms of the Creative
Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The
use, distribution or reproduction in other
forums is permitted, provided the original
author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are
credited and that the original publication in
this journal is cited, in accordance with
accepted academic practice. No use,
distribution or reproduction is permitted
which does not comply with these terms.
Cure for tantrums? Longitudinal
associations between parental
digital emotion regulation and
childrens self-regulatory skills
V. Konok
1
*
, M.-A. Binet
2
, Á. Korom
3
, Á. Pogány
1
, Á. Miklósi
1,4
and
C. Fitzpatrick
5
1
Department of Ethology, Faculty of Science, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary,
2
Faculty of
Medicine and Health Sciences, Université de Sherbrooke, Sherbrooke, QC, Canada,
3
Faculty of Science,
Doctoral School of Biology, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary,
4
ELKH-ELTE Comparative
Ethology Research Group, Budapest, Hungary,
5
Faculty of Education, Université de Sherbrooke,
Sherbrooke, QC, Canada
Introduction: Parents often use digital devices to regulate their childrens
negative emotions, e.g., to stop tantrums. However, this could hinder child
development of self-regulatory skills. The objective of the study was to
observe bidirectional longitudinal associations between parents reliance on
digital devices to regulate their childs emotions and self-regulatory tendencies
(anger/frustration management, effortful control, impulsivity).
Methods: Parents (N = 265) lled out the Child Behavior QuestionnaireShort
Form and the Media Assessment Questionnaire twice: the initial assessment
(T1) took place in 2020 (mean child age = 3.5 years old), and follow-up (T2)
occurred a year later in 2021 (mean child age = 4.5 years old).
Results: Higher occurrence of parental digital emotion regulation (PDER) in T1
predicts higher anger and lower effortful control in T2, but not impulsivity.
Higher anger in T1, but not impulsivity and effortful control, predicts higher
PDER in T2.
Discussion: Our results suggest that parents of children with greater
temperament-based anger use digital devices to regulate the childs emotions
(e.g., anger). However, this strategy hinders development of self-regulatory
skills, leading to poorer effortful control and anger management in the child.
KEYWORDS
emotion regulation, self-regulation, digital devices, longitudinal, effortful control,
impulsivity
1 Introduction
Digital device use among young children is markedly increasing. Children are
introduced to screens at earlier and earlier developmental stages (1). Screen-based
activities occupy the largest part of childrens free time, compared to outdoor play or
other screen-free activities (2). Pre-pandemic studies report that preschoolers spend
about 1.52.5 h in front of a screen daily (3, 4), and this amount has increased by one
hour during the pandemic (5). The proportion of families that possess mobile devices
has increased from 52% in 2011 to 98% in 2020 and almost half of 2- to 4-years-olds
have their own mobile device (6). About 26% of children aged 04 from the United
States spend more than 4 h in front of a screen daily, meaning a two-fold increase
compared to 13% before the pandemic (7). Despite these tendencies the effects of
TYPE Original Research
PUBLISHED 28 June 2024
|
DOI 10.3389/frcha.2024.1276154
Frontiers in Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 01 frontiersin.org
screen time on child emotional and cognitive development are still
debated and largely unknown (8). The widespread usage of
electronic media and digital devices (TV, videogames, PC,
smartphones, tablets, etc.) may inuence even adults cognition,
emotions, and mental health (9, 10). However, young childrens
brain and cognitive processes are still plastic, making them even
more potentially vulnerable to strong and long-lasting inuences
of experiences (1113).
Early childhood is a critical time for learning basic self-
regulation skills (14). Self-regulation is conceptualized as the
organization or modulation of affective, mental, and behavioral
responses, including control over emotional experiences and
expressions (i.e., emotion regulation), cognitive processes
(i.e., executive function), and approach or withdrawal behaviors
(i.e., effortful control) (15). Executive function and effortful
control are related and, according to some researchers,
overlapping constructs (16, 17). Effortful control has been
dened as childrens ability to inhibit a dominant response in
favor of a subdominant one, or an automatic response in favor
of a deliberate one (18, 19). It involves the management of
attention or behavior. Effortful control is temperamentally based,
but also develops with considerable input from the environment
(19) especially through childrens early social relationships with
their parents (20, 21).
Emotion regulation involves implicit or explicit attempts to
modify the natural trajectory of one or more parameters of
emotion (22), including physiological arousal, expression,
intensity or duration. It is related to temperamental emotional
reactivity [high reactivity hinders its effectiveness (23);], and to
coping [i.e., the ability to cope with the stressful situation
(24, 25)]. Emotion regulation emerges in rudimentary forms in
infancy (26), and gradually progresses from being a highly
external process to an internal one over time (25). Certain early
childhood experiences, e.g., appropriate family interactions are
necessary for this developmental process (27).
In the past decades, digital devices have become increasingly
prevalent in peoples lives and became objects with which
emotions, cognition, and behavior can be regulated. Therefore,
devices and screen-based activities have become external tools of
self-regulation. For example, digital activities (e.g., videogaming,
watching videos, instant messaging) often serve an emotion-
regulating purpose in adults [digital emotion regulation
;(28);
for a review: (29)]. They help in coping with or recovery from
negative emotions and stress by providing a sense of mastery and
control (29, 30), an immersive or ow experience (3133), and
by providing a distraction from real-life problems or escape from
reality into the virtual world (34). Digital activities offer instant
rewards (35, 36) which can modulate ones mood and emotions.
Digital activities can also help in arousal-regulation for
individuals with lower arousal by offering stimulation through
e.g., fast-paced, intensive, simultaneous stimuli (8, 37)or
arousing (e.g., violent) contents, which activate the dopamine
and the reward pathways (8).
Parents often give digital devices to their child to safely
engage them (baby-sitter function), and to regulate their
emotions or behavior (3840). Kabali et al. (41) found that 65%
of parents use mobile devices to keep their child calm in public
places. Television is also commonly used as a calming tool for
children (42, 43). We refer to this phenomenon as parental
digital emotion regulation (PDER), designating parental
behaviors such as giving the child a digital device to regulate
their negative emotions or calm them down.
Providing children with digital devices as digital paciers (41)
may reduce child emotional expressions in the short term and may
help parents allocate resources to necessary tasks and provide them
with free time, especially during lockdown (44). However, this
practice may also lead to missed opportunities to teach adaptive
emotion regulation and coping strategies to the child (45).
Although the distraction of attention away from stressful stimuli
or negative emotions can be an effective short-term strategy to
reduce emotional intensity in young children (46), suppressing
emotions can have paradoxical, rebounding effects [e.g., (47)],
and it may lead to maladaptive, avoidant coping strategies,
increased negative emotionality or dysregulation in the future.
Additionally, for children with immature self-regulatory skills, it
may be harmful to become accustomed to external devices to
regulate their emotions, as this could interfere with the
development of internal regulatory mechanisms. Dependence on
the device may lead to problematic media use and screen time
tantrums (48), i.e., extreme emotions when media is removed
(40). In addition, when digital devices are used for getting
instant rewards (35, 36), it may hinder ones ability to delay
gratication and control impulses (49, 50). This may lead to a
positive feedback loop. As a consequence, an association is
frequently found between media use and impulsivity (5153)or
poorer executive functions (5457).
Empirical evidence suggests a negative association between
digital media use and self-regulation. For example, more time
spent watching TV was associated with higher ratings on
negative emotionality, emotional reactivity, aggression, and
attention problems, as well as lower levels of soothability in
toddlers (58). Children who began using screen media devices
earlier or who spent more time engaging with mobile devices
displayed lower self-regulation (59). Longitudinal data mainly
suggest bidirectional relationships. For example, screen time or
digital media use at a younger age was negatively associated with
later child self-regulation or related processes, such as executive
function and effortful control (37, 55, 57, 6062). However,
ndings also support the reverse association: emotion
dysregulation and poor self-regulation was found to contribute to
greater and more problematic media use later (37, 42, 63, 64).
However, whether bidirectional associations exist between the use
of PDER and child self-regulation remains unknown.
The few studies investigating PDER suggest its potential role in
child self-regulatory skills development. Coyne et al. (40) found that
temperamental dysregulation risk factors, specically negative
affectivity and surgency (65, 66), were related to problematic
media use and screen time tantrums through PDER. This
suggests that difcult temperament (entailing low self-regulation
skills) leads to PDER, which, in turn, leads to problematic media
use. In line with this, children with social-emotional difculties,
poor self-regulation and a difcult temperament have a higher
Konok et al. 10.3389/frcha.2024.1276154
Frontiers in Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 02 frontiersin.org
chance of being given digital technology as a calming tool or as a
baby-sitter (39, 42, 43, 67, 68) and perhaps as a result, they use
more media later (37, 62, 64, 69).
However, as far as we know, only one longitudinal study (69)
investigated the bidirectional associations between PDER and
child self-regulation. This study found an interaction effect:
PDER in an earlier time point (T1) was positively related to
increases in childrens negative emotionality in a later time point
(T2), but only for children with initially low negative
emotionality. Further longitudinal studies are needed as the
above-mentioned results are not in line with the frequently
found bidirectional association between media use and self-
regulation. Therefore, the objective of the study was to observe
bidirectional longitudinal associations between PDER and child
self-regulatory tendencies (anger/frustration management,
effortful control, impulsivity). Since self-regulatory skills are still
immature in the preschool age, PDER can have a great impact
on them. Therefore, we aimed to investigate this age group. We
also accounted for the confounding effects of parenting stress,
child screen time, and family sociodemographics.
This is a conrmatory study with clear hypotheses: we expect
that poorer child self-regulatory tendencies (i.e., higher level of
anger/frustration management problems, impulsivity and lower
level of effortful control) (at T1) lead to higher PDER (at T2), and
that higher PDER (at T1) leads to poorer child self-regulatory
tendencies (higher anger/frustration management problems and
impulsivity, and lower level of effortful control) (at T2).
2 Materials and method
2.1 Procedure and participants
This study is part of a larger, two-year longitudinal study on
digital media use by Canadian families with preschool children
aged 25 years during the COVID-19 pandemic. Participating
families were recruited using eye-catching posters and yers in
preschools and pre-kindergarten classes, sign-up sheets and
presentations given at preschool and pre-kindergarten
registration nights, a Facebook page, and newspaper and radio
advertisements broadcast across Nova Scotia (Canada). To
measure bidirectional associations between parental digital
emotion regulation and child self-regulation tendencies, we
measured these variables at two time points.
The rst assessment took place between March and August of
2020 (N = 316 children), during a provincially declared state of
emergency and lockdown. A follow-up with this sample was
conducted a year later, between April and August of 2021 (N = 265;
a ow diagram of the participants is presented in Figure 1).
Demographics for the retested sample are presented in Table 1.
At both time waves, parents completed the web-based Media
Assessment Questionnaire (MAQ), which has been described in
detail elsewhere (70). The MAQ assesses child and parent media
use and includes questions on child age and sex, parent
education, as well as reasons reported by parents to allow their
child to use media. For the purpose of this study, we integrated
items on child temperamental anger/frustration, impulsivity and
effortful control using the Child Behavior QuestionnaireShort
Form (71). We also integrated items on parenting stress using
the Parenting Stress Index (72). These measures are described
below. The use of data for this specic study received approval
from the ethics board from the principal investigators institution
(IRB #2021-2927). Informed consent to participate was obtained
from participating parents.
2.2 Measures
2.2.1 Parental digital emotion regulation (PDER)
Parents were asked to rate how much they agreed or disagreed
with the statement I let my child use media to calm them down
when they are upset. Responses were rated on a 7-point Likert
scale ranging from Never (1) to Several times per day (7). Due to
some of the response options having very small frequencies (e.g.,
Several times per day: N = 4), this variable was then recoded
into a dichotomous variable (1 = Never/rarely, 2 = Regularly/
frequently; see Table 1 for descriptives).
2.2.2 Child self-regulatory tendencies
The Child Behavior QuestionnaireShort form (71) assesses
several distinct dimensions of temperament which are grouped
into three main factors: negative affectivity, surgency/
extraversion, and effortful control. The short form shows
satisfactory internal consistency, criterion validity, longitudinal
stability and inter-rater agreement (71, 73). Since we focus on
temperament-based self-regulatory tendencies, we retained for
this study (1) anger/frustration (anger hereinafter) which is a
dimension belonging to the negative affectivity main factor, (2)
impulsivity which is a dimension belonging to the surgency/
extraversion main factor, and nally (3) the main factor of
effortful control. Anger (e.g., Child gets angry when told s/he
has to go to bed) and impulsivity (e.g., Usually rushes into an
activity without thinking about it) were each based on the mean
FIGURE 1
Flow diagram showing the number of participants contacted, lost for
specic reasons and retained.
Konok et al. 10.3389/frcha.2024.1276154
Frontiers in Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 03 frontiersin.org
of 6 items. Higher scores in anger and impulsivity subscales
indicate greater intensity and duration of the childs angry or
frustrated response to environmental stimuli or greater speed of
response initiation, respectively. The effortful control factor was
based on mean scores obtained for the dimensions of attentional
focusing (six items, e.g., When drawing or coloring a book,
shows strong concentration) and inhibitory control (six items,
e.g., Can wait before entering into new activities if s/he is asked
to). Higher scores in the attentional focusing and the inhibitory
control subscales indicate better child effortful control. Items are
scored on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (extremely
untrue of your child) to 7 (extremely true of your child). The
Cronbachs alpha coefcients for anger, impulsivity, and effortful
control in Wave 1 were α = 0.789; 0.629 and 0.792, respectively.
In Wave 2, the corresponding coefcients were α = 0.814; 0.656
and 0.785, respectively (see Table 1 for descriptives).
2.2.3 Demographics, parenting stress and child
screen time
When completing the MAQ (70), parents reported child age
and sex (assigned at birth), parent age and sex, parent education,
yearly income, race, country of birth, marital status, and
parenting stress. Race, country of birth and marital status were
not used in the analyses because there was little variance on
these variables (see Table 1 for response options and number/
percentage of participants answering them). Parenting stress was
assessed using the Parenting Stress Index (72). This questionnaire
includes a Parental distress (PD) subscale (12 items, i.e., I nd
myself giving up more of my life to meet my childs needs than I
ever expected) and a Parent-child dysfunctional interaction
(PCDI) subscale (12 items, i.e., My child smiles at me much less
than I expected). Items were rated on a 5-point Likert scale as:
1 (strongly disagree); 2 (disagree); 3 (not sure); 4 (agree) or 5
(strongly agree) and were then averaged to create a total score
ranging from 1 to 5, with an adequate internal consistency
(Cronbachs alpha = 0.850). Higher scores indicate higher
parenting stress (see Table 1 for descriptives).
In the MAQ, parents also reported their child screen time by
reporting the average amount of time their child spent doing
each of the following activities: (1) Watching TV or DVDs; (2)
Using a computer; (3) Playing video games on a console; (4);
Using an iPad, tablet, LeapPad, iTouch, or similar mobile device
(excluding smartphones); or (5) Using a smartphone. For each
activity, response options were: (1) Never; (2) Less than 30 min;
(3) 30 min to 1 h; (4) 12 h; (5) 23 h; (6) 34 h; (7) 45 h; and
(7) more than 5 h. Parents reported this separately for a typical
weekday and a typical weekend day. Total amount of child
screen time was calculated by summing the durations for each
TABLE 1 Descriptive statistics of the nal sample (N = 265).
Variable Measure N (%) M ± SD (Min, Max)
Child age T1 In years N (%) missing = 1 (0.4) 3.46 ± 0.84 (2, 5.42)
Child age T2 In years N (%) missing = 0 4.33 ± 0.86 (2.75, 6.33)
Child sex (2 values) N (%) boys = 138 (52.1) N (%) girls = 126 (47.5) N (%) missing = 1 (0.4)
Parent age T1 In years N (%) missing = 0 (0) 35.24 ± 4.28 (23, 52)
Parent age T2 In years N (%) missing = 0 (0) 36.24 ± 4.28 (24, 53)
Parent sex (2 values) N (%) males = 20 (7.5) N (%) females = 245 (92.5) N (%) missing = 0 (0)
Parental education (3 values) N (%) college or secondary degree = 62 (23.4)
N (%) bachelors degree = 129 (48.7)
N (%) master or doctoral degree = 74 (27.9) N (%) missing = 0 (0)
Yearly income (3 values) N (%) $59,999 and less = 40 (15.1)
N (%) 60,000$99,999 = 72 (27.2)
N (%) $100,000 and more = 136 (51.3) N (%) missing = 17 (6.4)
Race (7 values) N (%) Aboriginal = 2 (0.8) N (%) Asian = 5 (1.9) N (%) Black = 2 (0.8) N (%) White = 242 (91.3)
N% Dont know = 0 (0) N (%) Prefer not to answer = 1 (0.4) N (%) Other = 12 (4.5) N (%)
missing = 1 (0.4)
Country of birth (2 values) N (%) Canada (country) = 239 (90.2) N (%) Other = 25 (9.4) N (%) missing = 1 (0.4)
Marital status (6 values) N (%) Married = 218 (82.3) N (%) Single/Never married = 11 (4.2) N (%) Live-in partner = 28
(10.6) N (%) Divorced = 2 (0.8) N (%) Widowed = 0 (0) N (%) Separated = 5 (1.) N (%)
missing = 1 (0.4)
Child screen time T1 In hours/day N (%) missing = 0 (0) 3.45 ± 2.45 (0, 10.43)
Child screen time T2 In hours/day N
(%) missing = 0 (0) 3.26 ± 2.38 (0, 9.65)
Parenting stress T1 (total score 05) N (%) missing = 0 (0) 1.85 ± 0.53 (1, 3.76)
Parenting stress T2 (total score 05) N (%) missing = 0 (0) 1.14 ± 0.72 (0, 3.11)
CBQ anger T1 (total score 07) N (%) missing = 0 (0) 4.25 ± 1.11 (1, 7)
CBQ anger T2 (total score 07) N (%) missing = 0 (0) 4.26 ± 1.15 (1, 6.67)
CBQ impulsivity T1 (total score 07) N (%) missing = 0 (0) 4.43 ± 0.93 (1.67, 7)
CBQ impulsivity T2 (total score 07) N (%) missing = 0 (0) 4.19 ± 0.94 (1.33, 6.83)
CBQ effortful control T1 (total score 07) N (%) missing = 0 (0) 4.71 ± 0.82 (2.58, 6.83)
CBQ effortful control T2 (total score 07) N (%) missing = 0 (0) 4.88 ± 0.82 (2.75, 7)
Parental digital emotion
regulation T1
(after merging:
2 values)
N (%) never/rarely = 165 (62.3) N (%) regularly/frequently = 100 (37.7) N (%) missing = 0 (0)
Parental digital emotion
regulation T2
(after merging:
2 values)
N (%) never/rarely = 179 (67.5) N (%) regularly/frequently = 86 (32.5) N (%) missing = 0 (0)
Konok et al. 10.3389/frcha.2024.1276154
Frontiers in Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 04 frontiersin.org
activity, using responses mid-points with the exception of Never
and more than 5 h where scores of 0 and 5 were used,
respectively. To compute child average daily screen time, we
computed a weighted average of screen time across the week as
follows: [(weekday screen time X 5) + (weekend screen time X
2)]/7 (see Table 1 for descriptives).
2.3 Data analytic strategy
First, we compared whether retained (those who participated at
T2) and unretained (those who had dropped out) participants were
different in any aspects of demographics, parental digital emotion
regulation (PDER), or child self-regulation scores (MannWhitney
tests, t-tests and χ
2
tests, using SPSS 28.0.0.0).
Of the nal retained sample of 265, 17 participants had a
missing value on income and one participant had a missing
value for child anger, impulsivity, and effortful control at T2. We
performed Littles test to determine if data were missing
completely at random (MCAR). The test was not signicant
revealing that data could be assumed to be MCAR: χ
2
= 24.476,
DF = 26, p = 0.549.
We imputed missing values on income to the median (value
of 3) and u sed FIML (maximum likelihood in for mation) to
account for missing outcome data.
To test bidirectional associations between PDER and self-
regulation scales, we estimated cross-lagged panel models using
Mplus version 8.10 (74). We controlled for sociodemographic
variables such as child age and sex, parent age and sex, parent
education, yearly income, child screen time and parenting stress
(the model schema is presented on Figure 2).
3 Results
3.1 Comparing retained and unretained
participants
Unretained participants were signicantly different from
retained participants in parents age (M ± SD = 33.32 ± 4.631
(unretained) vs. 35.226 ± 4.278 years (retained); t = 2.852; p = 0.005;
Cohens d = 0.436) and marginally in PDER (52% vs. 37.6% of
regular/frequent PDER in unretained and retained sample,
respectively; χ
2
= 3.643; p = 0.056; this is discussed in the limitation
section), but was not signicantly different on any other
demographic variables or child behavior variables (all p > 0.170).
3.2 Cross-lagged panel model: anger
Our nal model is presented in Figure 3. Our cross-lagged
panel model provided good t [CFI = 1.000; TLI = 1.000; RMSEA
= 0.000 (0.000; 0.138)] and accounted for 36.7% and 45.8% of
the variance in PDER and Anger at T2, respectively. Analyses
revealed considerable stability in PDER (b = 1.233; SE = 0.187;
p < 0.001; β = 0.476) and Anger (b = 0.567; SE = 0.054; p < 0.001;
β = 0.546) between T1 and T2. In terms of the cross-lagged
associations, T1 PDER signicantly contributed to higher Anger
at T2 (b = 0.304; SE = 0.122; p = 0.013; β = 0.128), whereas T1
Anger only tendentiously contributed to higher PDER at T2
(b = 0.180; SE = 0.1 08; p =0.094; β = 0.159). Parenting stress
(b = 0.018; SE = 0.007; p = 0.008; β = 0.124) and child s creen
time (b = 0.063; SE = 0.024; p = 0.009; β = 0.133) at T1 were also
signicantly positively associated with Anger at T2 (Table 2).
3.3 Cross-lagged panel model: impulsivity
Our model is presented in Figure 4. Our cross-lagged panel
model provided good t [CFI = 1.000; TLI = 1.000; RMSEA = 0.000
(0.000; 0.078)] and accounted for 34.7% and 47.8% of the variance
in PDER and Impulsivity at T2, respectively. Analyses revealed
considerable stability in Impulsivity (b = 0.670; SE = 0.051; p < 0.001;
β = 0.659) between T1 and T2. In terms of the cross-lagged
associations, neither T1 PDER was associated with T2 Impulsivity
(b = 0.064; SE = 0.094; p = 0.496; β = 0.033), nor T1 Impulsivity
with T2 PDER (b = 0.019; SE = 0.096; p =0.845; β = 0.014). Child
age was signicantly negatively associated with Impulsivity at T2
(b = 0.126; SE = 0.057; p = 0.028; β = 0.112; Table 3).
FIGURE 2
Summary of the cross-lagged panel analyses.
Konok et al. 10.3389/frcha.2024.1276154
Frontiers in Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 05 frontiersin.org
3.4 Cross-lagged panel model: effortful
control
Our model is presented in Figure 5. Our cross-lagged panel
model provided good t [CFI = 1.000; TLI = 1.000; RMSEA = 0.000
(0.000; 0.000)] and accounted for 34.7% and 55.5% of the variance
in PDER and Effortful control at T2, respectively. Analyses
revealed considerable stability in Effortful control between T1 and
T2 (b = 0.716; SE = 0.050; p <0.001; β = 0.718). In terms of the
cross-lagged associations, T1 PDER signicantly contributed to
lower Effortful control at T2 (b = 0.182; SE = 0.075; p =0.016;
β = 0.108), whereas T1 Effortful control did not contribute to
higher PDER at T2 (b = 0.002; SE = 0.129; p = 0.986; β = 0.001).
Parent age was also signicantly negatively associated with Effortful
control at T2 (b = 0.019; SE = 0.008; p = 0.022; β = 0.098; Table 4).
4 Discussion
We investigated the relationships between parental digital
emotion regulation and self-regulation in children. Our study
revealed complex, bidirectional longitudinal associations between
TABLE 2 Results of the cross-lagged panel model measuring the bi-
directional associations between PDER (parental digital emotion
regulation) and the anger/frustration dimension of the child behavior
questionnaire.
Estimate (b) se p-value Beta
Child age T2 PDER 0.168 0.118 0.155 0.112
Child sex T2 PDER 0.064 0.190 0.737 0.025
T1 screen time T2 PDER 0.054 0.043 0.209 0.105
Parent age T2 PDER 0.022 0.025 0.359 0.077
Parent sex T2 PDER 0.018 0.382 0.962 0.004
Parent education T2 PDER 0.127 0.146 0.384 0.073
Yearly income T2 PDER 0.048 0.124 0.700 0.028
T1 parenting stress T2 PDER 0.002 0.013 0.870 0.013
T1 PDER T2 PDER 1.233 0.187 0.000 0.476
T1 Anger T2 PDER 0.180 0.108 0.094 0.159
Child age T2 Anger 0.021 0.072 0.768 0.015
Child sex T2 Anger 0.066 0.113 0.558 0.029
T1 screen time T2 Anger 0.063 0.024 0.009 0.133
Parent age T2 Anger 0.020 0.014 0.153 0.073
Parent sex T2 Anger 0.314 0.178 0.078 0.072
Parent education T2 Anger 0.066 0.082 0.425 0.041
Yearly income T2 Anger 0.036 0.081 0.658 0.023
T1 parenting stress T2 Anger 0.018 0.007 0.008 0.124
T1 anger T2 Anger 0.567 0.054 0.000 0.546
T1 PDER T2 Anger 0.304 0.122 0.013 0.128
FIGURE 3
Longitudinal cross-lagged associations between parental digital emotion regulation and the anger/frustration dimension of the child behavior
questionnaire.
FIGURE 4
Longitudinal cross-lagged associations between parental digital emotion regulation and the impulsivity dimension of the child behavior questionnaire.
Konok et al. 10.3389/frcha.2024.1276154
Frontiers in Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 06 frontiersin.org
the investigated variables. The results suggest that parental digital
emotion regulation may contribute to the bidirectional
association between media use and self-regulation in children
(37, 55, 62, 63). The observed associations were consistent and
strong in one direction (higher frequency of parental digital
emotion regulation leading to higher anger/frustration and lower
effortful control), but less consistent and more tendentious in the
other direction (effortful control does not, while anger/frustration
tendentiously contribute to higher PDER).
4.1 Higher PDER leads to poorer
anger/frustration management and
effortful control
Higher baseline occurrence of parental digital emotion
regulation (PDER) and higher baseline screen time predicted
poorer anger/frustration management (i.e., higher anger) one
year later. This is in line with ndings of a cross-sectional study
(hence limitations regarding causal inferences) that more time
spent watching TV is associated with higher levels of negative
emotionality, emotional reactivity, and aggression, as well as
lower levels of soothability in toddlers (58). Longitudinal studies
(57, 62) found that baseline digital media use predicted more
externalizing problems (specically, conduct problems and
hyperactivity) at follow-up, and these problems often entail
difculties with anger management (7578). While these
associations or effects can be driven by several mechanisms [e.g.,
direct effects, like overstimulation, and indirect effects, like
displacement of social interactions (79)], the present study
suggests that using digital devices for emotion regulation might
be a key determinant in the development of child difculties
with various aspects of self-regulation. Our results somewhat
contradict those of Gordon-Hacker & Gueron-Sela (69), who
found in a path analysis that early maternal digital emotion
regulation preceded later negative emotionality only in children
FIGURE 5
Longitudinal cross-lagged associations between parental digital emotion regulation and the effortful control main factor of the child behavior
questionnaire.
TABLE 3 Results of the cross-lagged panel model measuring the bi-directional associations between PDER (parental digital emotion regulation) and the
impulsivity dimension of the child behavior questionnaire.
Estimate (b) se p-value Beta
Child age T2 PDER 0.167 0.118 0.158 0.114
Child sex T2 PDER 0.059 0.189 0.755 0.024
T1 Screen time T2 PDER 0.064 0.043 0.138 0.127
Parent age T2 PDER 0.025 0.025 0.304 0.087
Parent sex T2 PDER 0.007 0.395 0.986 0.001
Parent education T2 PDER 0.117 0.145 0.422 0.068
Yearly income T2 PDER 0.037 0.121 0.761 0.022
T1 parenting stress T2 PDER 0.006 0.012 0.597 0.039
T1 PDER T2 PDER 1.276 0.185 0.000 0.501
T1 Impulsivity T2 PDER 0.019 0.096 0.845 0.014
Child age T2 Impulsivity 0.126 0.057 0.028 0.112
Child sex T2 Impulsivity 0.122 0.087 0.160 0.065
T1 screen time T2 Impulsivity 0.022 0.021 0.297 0.057
Parent age T2 Impulsivity 0.004 0.012 0.761 0.016
Parent sex T2 Impulsivity 0.203 0.145 0.164 0.057
Parent education T2 Impulsivity 0.103 0.064 0.106 0.079
Yearly income T2 Impulsivity 0.083 0.066 0.207 0.065
T1 parenting stress T2 Impulsivity 0.004 0.005 0.491 0.031
T1 Impulsivity T2 Impulsivity 0.670 0.051 0.000 0.659
T1 PDER T2 Impulsivity
0.064 0.094 0.496 0.033
Konok et al. 10.3389/frcha.2024.1276154
Frontiers in Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 07 frontiersin.org
with low initial negative emotionality. However, it should be noted
that the authors found a signicant, although weak (r = 0.2)
longitudinal correlation between T1 maternal digital emotion
regulation and T2 negative emotionality, and a slightly stronger
cross-sectional correlation (r = 0.37) between T2 maternal digital
emotion regulation and T2 negative emotionality, but neither of
them were signicant in the path analysis. One possible
explanation for the divergent ndings in their study and ours, is
the different age groups of the children. Additional explanation
for these somewhat contradictory results should be revealed
through further longitudinal studies. Furthermore, screen time
and PDER are closely related, and as the present design does
not allow for the separation of the two phenomena, further
research is needed to disentangle their respective effects on
child self-regulation.
Higher occurrence of PDER at T1 also predicted lower levels of
effortful control at T2. In line with this, longitudinal studies have
found that those who spend more time using digital devices
subsequently develop more attentional problems, impulsivity, and
poorer executive functions or self-regulation in general (37, 55,
61, 64, 80). These results corroborate the involvement of PDER
in developing self-regulation problems. Contrary to our
expectations, however, PDER in T1 did not predict impulsivity in
T2. This contrasts with the ndings of several studies which
showed that digital device use leads to hyperactivity, inattention
or externalizing behaviors (81). It was also unexpected that T1
PDER predicted only effortful control, whereas impulsivity and
effortful control are related constructs (16, 17). The scale of
effortful control is made up of items on attentional focusing (e.g.,
Tendency to maintain attentional focus upon task-related
channels) and inhibitory control (e.g., The capacity to plan and
to suppress inappropriate approach responses under instructions
or in novel or uncertain situations). On the other hand,
impulsivity is dened as the speed of response initiation (71),
consisting of items like Usually rushes into an activity without
thinking about it. While high impulsivity entails low inhibitory
control, and both are related to behavioral self-regulation,
attentional focusing is a different, more cognitive construct and
does not necessarily correlate with the other two (82, 83). It is
possible that PDER affects attentional processes inherent to
effortful control to a larger extent than behavioral self-regulation.
Higher PDER is associated with higher screen time (38) and the
latter may have negative effects on attentional focusing (37, 80),
for example, as a result of overstimulation (84). The associations
between early digital media use and later attentional problems
are well supported by empirical data (37, 80), while relationships
between digital media use and executive functions are more
contradictory (54, 56, 61, 8587). Therefore, further studies
should investigate the longitudinal associations of PDER with
attentional focusing and inhibitory control separately.
4.2 Poorer anger/frustration management
skills in T1, but not impulsivity and effortful
control, predicts tendentiously higher
occurrence of parental digital emotion
regulation in T2
Poorer baseline anger/frustration management skills (i.e.,
higher anger) tendentiously predicted higher occurrence of PDER
at follow-up. This result is in line with cross-sectional ndings
showing that children with social-emotional difculties, poor
self-regulation and a more difcult temperament have a higher
chance of being given digital technology as a calming tool or as a
baby-sitter (39, 43, 67, 68) and with longitudinal studies showing
that these problems lead to using more media later (37, 42,
TABLE 4 Results of the cross-lagged panel model measuring the bi-directional associations between PDER (parental digital emotion regulation) and the
effortful control main factor of the child behavior questionnaire.
Estimate (b) se p-value Beta
Child age T2 PDER 0.168 0.120 0.160 0.115
Child sex T2 PDER 0.061 0.189 0.749 0.024
T1 Screen time T2 PDER 0.066 0.042 0.118 0.130
Parent age T2 PDER 0.025 0.025 0.303 0.088
Parent sex T2 PDER 0.008 0.395 0.983 0.002
Parent education T2 PDER 0.117 0.146 0.423 0.068
Yearly income T2 PDER 0.039 0.120 0.749 0.023
T1 parenting stress T2 PDER 0.006 0.012 0.618 0.038
T1 PDER T2 PDER 1.275 0.186 0.000 0.500
T1 effortful control T2 PDER 0.002 0.129 0.986 0.001
Child age T2 Effortful control 0.066 0.048 0.164 0.068
Child sex T2 Effortful control 0.091 0.071 0.202 0.055
T1 screen time T2 Effortful control 0.015 0.016 0.349 0.044
Parent age T2 Effortful control 0.019 0.008 0.022 0.098
Parent sex T2 Effortful control 0.108 0.139 0.437 0.035
Parent education T2 Effortful control 0.013 0.049 0.788 0.012
Yearly income T2 Effortful control 0.039 0.048 0.418 0.035
T1 parenting stress T2 Effortful control 0.005 0.006 0.383 0.046
T1 effortful control T2 Effortful control 0.716 0.050 0.000 0.718
T1 PDER T2 Effortful control
0.182 0.075 0.016 0.108
Konok et al. 10.3389/frcha.2024.1276154
Frontiers in Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 08 frontiersin.org
62, 64). Our study is the rst longitudinal study in support of poor
emotion regulation leading to higher chances of parental digital
emotion regulation, although the association was only marginally
signicant (p = 0.094). Parents with difcult children may
struggle more with decreasing the tempers or negative emotions
of the child. Therefore, they may turn to digital devices to
alleviate their burden. As Radesky et al. (39) pointed out,
frustration with the childs behavior would lead to use of digital
media as a coping strategy (p. 397). Similarly, instrumental use
of media (using media as a behavior modier or as a babysitter)
was primarily endorsed by parents who are less condent about
their parenting (68), and children with difcult temperament
may contribute to parents being less condent about their
parenting skills (88, 89).
Impulsivity and effortful control did not predict later PDER.
This suggests that parents use digital media as a parenting tool
only for managing emotional self-regulation problems in the
child, but not cognitive or behavioral self-regulation problems.
This result is unexpected, but may reect the fact that
impulsivity and lower effortful control in the child may be less
challenging for the parent than anger management problems, as
the latter entails emotional outburst and tantrums. Some studies
(67, 68) indicate that impulsivity and lower effortful control
(specically, conduct problems and energetic temperaments) are
associated with PDER. However these studies are cross-sectional
and cannot inform causal nor directional inferences. The present
longitudinal study suggests that these self-regulatory tendencies
do not lead to PDER, but rather the other way around as we
found that PDER led to lower effortful control. Although many
longitudinal studies found that children with attentional
problems, higher impulsivity and lower self-regulation at baseline
consume more digital media later (37, 62, 64, 79), these effects
may not be driven by parental motivation to regulate the childs
behavior/emotion by digital devices. Based on our results, we
argue it is likely that children with these problems are more
prone to use digital devices, independently of how much their
parents try to regulate their behavior with the device.
4.3 Limitations
To draw appropriate conclusions from the results, some
limitations should be addressed.
PDER was solely measured by parent report and with only one
item. More elaborate measures are required in future studies to
corroborate the present ndings, and parent reported PDER
should be validated by behavioral observations. Parents also
reported child self-regulation tendencies, which could lead to
shared measurement bias. Replications with reports from
preschool teachers or using different methodologies engaging
parents more actively to support their recall memories or
opinions about their childs behavior could advance future studies.
The internal consistency for impulsivity was lower than
desirable (Cronbachs alpha was 0.629 in Wave 1 and 0.656 in
Wave 2). This might have reduced the statistical power of the
analysis to detect associations with PDER. Additionally, the
dimensions of impulsivity and anger have rarely been used
separately (90). Although their reliability has been frequently
proven to be satisfactory (71, 73), the validity of these subscales
is less known with few existing studies showing moderate
correlations with other questionnaire scales (91), and low to
moderate correlations with laboratory observational measures
(92, 93). In the future, more studies are required to better
corroborate the construct validity of these subscales.
Another potential confounder of the results is that data
collection took place during a provincially declared state of
emergency and lockdown because of the Covid-19 pandemic.
Since digital device use increased during lockdowns (94, 95), our
ndings should be replicated in post pandemic contexts.
Additionally, convenience sampling may not be representative
of the general population. This decreases the generalizability of the
results to the whole population. Replications with larger sample are
warranted. On the other hand, random sampling makes it
impossible to separate the effects of screen time and PDER (as
they are closely related). Therefore, further research is needed to
disentangle their effects on self-regulation.
Lastly, the unretained sample differed from the retained sample
in that parents in the unretained sample were younger and
marginally used more PDER than the retained participants. This
might have caused a systematic bias in the sample. Those who
participated in the second data collection wave might be more
conscious parents (applying less PDER), and this may distort the
observed associations. For example, it is possible that the
association between T1 effortful control and impulsivity and T2
PDER was not signicant (and between T1 anger and T2 PDER
was only marginally signicant) because conscious parents try to
nd other ways besides digital media to regulate or engage the
child. These shortcomings should be addressed in future studies.
4.4 Conclusion
Our study is the rst longitudinal study revealing bidirectional
associations between parental digital emotion regulation and child
emotion-regulation skills. Results support that higher anger/
frustration in the child renders parents tendentiously more likely
use digital devices to regulate child emotions. However, while
digital emotion regulation can be effective in the short term, this
strategy may hinder child development of self-regulatory skills in
the long term, leading to poorer effortful control and anger
management. This process may lead to a positive feedback loop,
resulting in increased dependence on the digital device and
potential later problematic media use, screen time tantrums
(40), and technological addiction (96). Based on these results,
efforts should be made to call parents attention on the harmful
consequences of digital emotion regulation. Pediatricians, child
psychologists, health professionals, and social workers working
directly with families or performing home visits should ask
parents about the use of digital media in the family. Additionally,
they should be especially attentive to parents of children with
difcult temperament, as they may be at higher risk of using
PDER. These parents should receive as much support as possible
Konok et al. 10.3389/frcha.2024.1276154
Frontiers in Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 09 frontiersin.org
to reinforce emotion regulation methods other then PDER. In
addition, peoples awareness should be increased about digital
devices being inappropriate tools for curing tantrums.
Data availability statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be
made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics statement
The studies involving humans were approved by Université de
Sherbrookes IRB #2021-2927. The studies were conducted in
accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements.
The participants provided their written informed consent to
participate in this study.
Author contributions
VK: Writing review & editing, Writing original draft,
Visualization, Funding acquisition, Formal Analysis,
Conceptualization. M-AB: Project administration, Writing
review & editing, Writing original draft. ÁK: Writing review
& editing, Writing original draft. ÁP: Writing review &
editing, Funding acquisition. ÁM: Writing review & editing,
Funding acquisition. CF: Writing review & editing,
Methodology, Funding acquisition, Conceptualization.
Funding
The authors declare that nancial support was received for the
research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
The study was funded by the Canadian Institutes of Health
Research (CIHR), Social sciences and humanities research
council (SSHRC), Nova Scotia Research (NSR), and the Canada
Research Chairs program (CRC), the National Research,
Development and Innovation Ofce (OTKA K 135478; OTKA
PD 134984), the Hungarian Academy of Sciences (MTA post-
covid 2021-50; Bolyai János Research Fellowship; MTA 01 031)
and the European Union project RRF-2.3.1-21-2022-00004
(Articial Intelligence National Laboratory). AP was funded by
the Hungarian Ethology Foundation.
Conict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the
absence of any commercial or nancial relationships that could
be construed as a potential conict of interest.
Publishers note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the
authors and do not necessarily represent those of their afliated
organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the
reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or
claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed
or endorsed by the publisher.
References
1. Konok V, Bunford N, Miklósi Á. Associations between child mobile use and
digital parenting style in Hungarian families. J Child Media. (202 0) 14(1):91109.
doi: 10.1080/17482798.2019.1684332
2. Gallup. Time to play: a study on childrens free time: how it is spent, proritized
and valued (2017). Available online at: https://news.gallup.com/reports/214853/time-
play-study-children-free-time-spent-prioritized-valued.aspx (Accessed February 02,
2023).
3. Carson V, Ezeugwu VE, Tamana SK, Chikuma J, Lefebvre DL, Azad MB, et al.
Associations between meeting the Canadian 24-hour movement guidelines for the
early years and behavioral and emotional problems among 3-year-olds. J Sci Med
Sport. (2019) 22(7):797802. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2019.01.003
4. Madigan S, Racine N, Tough S. Prevalence of preschoolers meeting vs exceeding
screen time guidelines. JAMA Pediatr. (2020) 174(1):935. doi: 10.1001/
jamapediatrics.2019.4495
5. Madigan S, Eirich R, Pador P, McArthur BA, Neville RD. Assessment of changes
in child and adolescent screen time during the COVID-19 pandemic : a systematic
review and meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatr. (2022) 176(12):118898. doi: 10.1001/
jamapediatrics.2022.4116
6. Rideout V, Robb MB. The Common Sense Census: Media Use by Kids Age Zero to
Eight, 2020. San Francisco, CA: Common Sense Media (2020).
7. Newall M, Machi S. Parents try to limit childrens screen time as it increases
during pandemic. Ipsos (2020). Available online at: https://www.ipsos.com/en-us/
parents-try-limit-childrens-screen-time-it-increases-during-pandemic (Accessed
February 10, 2023).
8. Lissak G. Adverse physiological and psychological effects of screen time on
children and adolescents: literature review and case study. Environ Res. (2018)
164:14957. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2018.01.015
9. Green CS, Bavelier D. Exercising your brain: a review of human brain plasticity
and training-induced learning. Psychol Aging. (2008) 23(4):692701. doi: 10.1037/
a0014345
10. Tromholt M. The facebook experiment: quitting facebook leads to higher levels
of well-being. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. (2016) 19(11):6616. doi: 10.1089/cyber.
2016.0259
11. Takeuchi H, Taki Y, Hashizume H, Asano K, Asano M, Sassa Y, et al. The
impact of television viewing on brain structures: cross-sectional and longitudinal
analyses. Cereb Cortex. (2015) 25(5):118897. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht315
12. Horowitz-Kraus T, Hutton JS. Brain connectivity in children is increased by the
time they spend reading books and decreased by the length of exposure to screen-
based media. Acta Paediatr Int J Paediatr. (2018) 107(4):68593. doi: 10.1111/apa.
14176
13. Hutton JS, Dudley J, DeWitt T, Horowitz-Kraus T. Associations between digital
media use and brain surface structural measures in preschool-aged children. Sci Rep.
(2022) 12(1):19095.
14. Shin M, Kemps E. Media multitasking as an avoidance coping strategy against
emotionally negative stimuli. Anxiety Stress Coping. (2020) 33(4):44051. doi: 10.
1080/10615806.2020.1745194
15. Jahromi LB. Self-regulation in young children with autism spectrum disorder:
an interdisciplinary perspective on emotion regulation, executive function,
and effortful control. Int Rev Res Dev Disabil. (2017) 53:4589. doi: 10.1016/bs.
irrdd.2017.07.007
16. Holzman JB, Bridgett DJ. Heart rate variability indices as bio-markers of top-
down self-regulatory mechanisms: a meta-analytic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev.
(2017) 74:233
55. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.12.032
Konok et al. 10.3389/frcha.2024.1276154
Frontiers in Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 10 frontiersin.org
17. Nigg JT. Annual research review: on the relations among self-regulation, self-
control, executive functioning, effortful control, cognitive control, impulsivity, risk-
taking, and inhibition for developmental psychopathology. J Child Psychol
Psychiatry. (2017) 58(4):36183. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12675
18. Kochanska G, Murray KT, Harlan ET. Effortful control in early childhood:
continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social development. Dev
Psychol. (2000) 36(2):220. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.36.2.220
19. Rothbart MK, Bates JE. Temperament. In: Eisenberg N, Damon W, Lerner RM,
editors. Handbook of Child Psychology: Social, Emotional, and Personality
Development. New Jersey: John Wiley & Sons, Inc (2006). p. 99166. https://
psycnet.apa.org/record/2006-08776-000 (Accessed February 17, 2023).
20. Eisenberg N, Smith CL, Spinrad TL. Effortful control: relations with emotion
regulation, adjustment, and socialization in childhood. In: Vohs KD, Baumeister RF,
editors. Handbook of Self-Regulation: Research, Theory, and Applications. 3rd ed.
New York: Guilford Press (2017). p. 45878.
21. Lengua LJ, Honorado E, Bush NR. Contextual risk and parenting as predictors of
effortful control and social competence in preschool children. J Appl Dev Psychol.
(2007) 28(1):4055. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2006.10.001
22. Gross JJ. Emotion regulation: current status and future prospects. Psychol Inq.
(2015) 26(1):126. doi: 10.1080/1047840X.2014.940781
23. Stifter CA, Spinrad TL. The effect of excessive crying on the development of
emotion regulation. Infancy. (2002) 3(2):13352. doi: 10.1207/S15327078IN0302_2
24. Cole PM, Martin SE, Dennis TA. Emotion regulation as a scientic construct:
methodological challenges and directions for child development research. Child Dev.
(2004) 75(2):31733. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00673.x
25. Thompson RA. Emotion regulation: a theme in search of denition. Monogr Soc
Res Child Dev. (1994) 59(23):2552. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5834 .1994.tb01276.x
26. Stifter CA. Individual differences in emotion regulation in infancy: a thematic
collection. Infancy. (2002) 3(2):12932. doi: 10.1207/S15327078IN0302_1
27. Morris AS, Silk JS, Steinberg L, Myers SS, Robinson LR. The role of the family
context in the development of emotion regulation. Soc Dev. (2007) 16(2):36188.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00389.x
28. Wadley G, Smith W, Koval P, Gross JJ. Digital emotion regulation. Curr Dir
Psychol Sci. (2020) 29(4):4128. doi: 10.1177/0963721420920592
29. Villani D, Carissoli C, Triberti S, Marchetti A, Gilli G, Riva G. Videogames for
emotion regulation: a systematic review. Games Health J. (2018) 7(2):8599. doi: 10.
1089/g4h.2017.0108
30. Sonnentag S, Fritz C. The recovery experience questionnaire: development and
validation of a measure for assessi ng recuperation and unwinding from work. J Occup
Health Psychol. (2007) 12(3):20421. doi: 10.1037/1076-8998.12.3.204
31. Reinecke L. Games and recovery. J Media Psychol. (2009a) 21(3):126
42. doi: 10.
1027/1864-1105.21.3.126
32. Reinecke L. Games at work: the recreational use of computer games during
working hours. Cyberpsychol Behav. (2009b) 12(4):4615. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2009.0010
33. Weber R, Tamborini R, Westcott-Baker A, Kantor B. Theorizing ow and media
enjoyment as cognitive synchronization of attentional and reward networks. Commun
Theory. (2009) 19(4):397422. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2885.2009.01352.x
34. Yee N. Motivations for play in online games. Cyberpsychol Behav. (2006) 9
(6):7725. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2006.9.772
35. Koepp MJ, Gunn RN, Lawrence AD, Cunningham VJ, Dagher A, Jones T, et al.
Evidence for striatal dopamine release during a video game. Nature. (1998) 393
(6682):2668. doi: 10.1038/30498
36. Han DH, Lee YS, Yang KC, Kim EY, Lyoo IK, Renshaw PF. Dopamine genes and
reward dependence in adolescents with excessive internet video game play. J Addict
Med. (2007) 1(3):1338. doi: 10.1097/ADM.0b013e31811f465f
37. Gentile DA, Swing EL, Lim CG, Khoo A. Video game playing, attention
problems, and impulsiveness: evidence of bidirectional causality. Psychol Pop Media
Cult. (2012) 1(1):6270. doi: 10.1037/a0026969
38. Beyens I, Eggermont S. Putting young children in front of the television:
antecedents and outcomes of parents use of television as a babysitter. Commun Q.
(2014) 62(1):5774. doi: 10.1080/01463373.2013.860904
39. Radesky JS, Peacock-Chambers E, Zuckerman B, Silverstein M. Use of mobile
technology to calm upset children. JAMA Pediatr. (2016) 170(4):397. doi: 10.1001/
jamapediatrics.2015.4260
40. Coyne SM, Shawcroft J, Gale M, Gentile DA, Etherington JT, Holmgren H, et al.
Tantrums, toddlers and technology: temperament, media emotion regulation, and
problematic media use in early childhood. Comput Human Behav. (2021) 120
(February):106762. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2021.106762
41. Kabali HK, Irigoyen MM, Nunez-Davis R, Budacki JG, Mohanty SH, Leister KP,
et al. Exposure and use of mobile media devices by young children. Pediatrics. (2015)
136(6):104450. doi: 10.1542/peds.2015-2151
42. Radesky JS, Silverstein M, Zuckerman B, Christakis DA. Infant self-regulation
and early childhood media exposure. Pediatrics. (2014) 133(5):e11728. doi: 10.
1542/peds.2013-2367
43. Thompson AL, Adair LS, Bentley ME. Maternal characteristics and perception of
temperament associated with infant TV exposure. Pediatrics. (2013) 131(2):e3907.
doi: 10.1542/peds.2012-1224
44. Ar undell L, Gould L, Ridgers ND, Ayala AMC, Downing KL, Salmon J, et al.
Everything kind of revolves around technology: a qualitative exploration of
families screen use experiences, and intervention suggestions. BMC Public Health.
(2022) 22(1):1606. doi: 10.1186/s12889-022-14007-w
45. Roberts MZ, Flagg AM, Lin B. Context matters: how smartphone (mis)use may
disrupt early emotion regulation development. New Ideas Psychol. (2022) 64(March
2021):100919. doi: 10.1016/j.newideapsych.2021.100919
46. Davis EL, Quiñones-Camacho LE, Buss KA. The effects of distraction and
reappraisal on childrens parasympathetic regulation of sadness and fear. J Exp
Child Psychol. (2016) 142:34458. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.09.020
47. Gross JJ, Levenson RW. Emotional suppression: physiology, self-report, and
expressive behavior. J Pers Soc Psychol . (1993) 64(6):97086. doi: 10.1037/0022-
3514.64.6.970
48. Hiniker A, Suh H, Cao S, Kientz JA. Screen time tantrums. Proceedings of the
2016 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (2016). p. 64860.
doi: 10.1145/2858036.2858278
49. Hadar AA, Eliraz D, Lazarovits A, Alyagon U, Zangen A. Using longitudinal
exposure to causally link smartphone usage to changes in behavior, cognition and
right prefrontal neural activity. Brain Stimul. (2015) 8(2):318. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.
2015.01.032
50. Wilmer HH, Chein JM. Mobile technology habits: patterns of association among
device usage, intertemporal preference, impulse control, and reward sensitivity.
Psychon Bull Rev. (2016) 23(5):160714. doi: 10.3758/s13423-016-1011-z
51. Minear M, Brasher F, McCurdy M, Lewis J, Younggren A. Working memory,
uid intelligence, and impulsiveness in heavy media multitaskers. Psychon Bull Rev.
(2013) 20(6):127481. doi: 10.3758/s13423-013-0456-6
52. Sanbonmatsu DM, Strayer DL, Medeiros-Ward N, Watson JM. Who multi-tasks
and why? Multi-tasking ability, perceived multi-tasking ability, impulsivity,
and sensation seeking. PLoS One. (2013) 8(1):e54402. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.
0054402
53. Shih S-I. A null relationship between media multitasking and well-being. PLoS
One. (2013) 8(5):e64508. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0064508
54. Nathanson AI, Aladé F, Sharp ML, Rasmussen EE, Christy K. The relation
between television exposure and executive function among preschoolers. Dev
Psychol. (2014) 50(5):1497506. doi: 10.1037/a0035714
55. McHarg G, Ribner AD, Devine RT, Hughes C. Screen time and executive
function in toddlerhood: a longitu dinal study. Front Psychol. (2020b) 11:570392.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.570392
56. Corkin MT, Peterson ER, Henderson AME, Waldie KE, Reese E, Morton SMB.
Preschool screen media exposure, executive functions and symptoms of
inattention/hyperactivity. J Appl Dev Psychol. (2021) 73:101237. doi: 10.1016/j.
appdev.2020.101237
57. McNeill J, Howard SJ, Vella SA, Cliff DP. Longitudinal associations of electronic
application use and media program viewing with cognitive and psychosocial
development in preschoolers. Acad Pediatr. (2019) 19(5):520 8. doi: 10.1016/j.acap.
2019.02.010
58. Desmarais E, Brown K, Campbell K, French BF, Putnam SP, Casalin S, et al.
Links between television exposure and toddler dysregulation: does culture matter?
Infant Behav Dev. (2021) 63(March):101557. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2 021.101557
59. Lawrence AC, Narayan MS, Choe DE. Association of young childrens use of
mobile devices with their self-regulation. JAMA Pediatr. (2020) 174(8):7935.
American Medical Association. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2020.0129
60. Cerniglia L, Cimino S, Ammaniti M. What are the effects of screen time on
emotion regulation and academic achievements? A three-wave longitudinal study on
children from 4 to 8 years of age. J Early Child Res. (2021) 19(2):14560. doi: 10.
1177/1476718X20969846
61. McHarg G, Ribner AD, Devine RT, Hughes C. Infant screen exposure links to
toddlers
inhibition, but not other EF constructs: a propensity score study. Infancy.
(2020a) 25(2):20522. doi: 10.1111/infa.12325
62. Poulain T, Vogel M, Neef M, Abicht F, Hilbert A, Genuneit J, et al.
Reciprocal associations between electronic media use and behavioral difculties in
preschoolers. Int J Environ Res Public Health . (2018) 15(4):814. doi: 10.3390/
ijerph15040814
63. Paulus FW, Hübler K, Mink F, Möhler E. Emotional dysregulation in preschool
age predicts later media use and gaming disorder symptoms in childhood. Front
Psychiatry. (2021) 12(June):111. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.626387
64. Cliff DP, Howard SJ, Radesky JS, McNeill J, Vella SA. Early childhood media
exposure and self-regulation: bidirectional longitudinal associations. Acad Pediatr.
(2018) 18(7):8139. doi: 10.1016/j.acap.2018.04.012
65. Spinrad TL, Stifter CA. Toddlers empathy-related responding to distress:
predictions from negative emotionality and maternal behavior in infancy. Infancy.
(2006) 10(2):97121. doi: 10.1207/s15327078in1002_1
Konok et al. 10.3389/frcha.2024.1276154
Frontiers in Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 11 frontiersin.org
66. Hagan MJ, Luecken LJ, Modecki KL, Sandler IN, Wolchik SA. Childhood
negative emotionality predicts biobehavioral dysregulation 15 years later. Emotion.
(2016) 16(6):87785. doi: 10.1037/emo0000161
67. Nabi RL, Krcmar M. It takes two: the effect of child characteristics on U.S.
parents motivations for allowing electronic media use. J Child Media. (2016) 10
(3):285303. doi: 10.1080/17482798.201 6.1162185
68. Nikken P. Parents instrumental use of media in childrearing: relationships with
condence in parenting, and health and conduct problems in children. J Child Fam
Stud. (2019) 28(2):53146. doi: 10.1007/s10826-018-1281-3
69. Gordon-Hacker A, Gueron-Sela N. Maternal use of media to regulate child
distress: a double-edged sword? Longitudinal links to toddlers negative
emotionality. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. (2020) 23(6):4005. doi: 10.1089/cyber.
2019.0487
70. Barr R, Kirkorian H, Radesky J, Coyne S, Nichols D, Blancheld O, et al. Beyond
screen time: a synergistic approach to a more comprehensive assessment of family
media exposure during early childhood. Front Psychol. (2020) 11:1283. doi: 10.3389/
fpsyg.2020.01283
71. Putnam SP, Rothbart MK. Development of short and very short forms of the
childrens behavior questionnaire. J Pers Assess. (2006) 87(1):10212. doi: 10.1207/
s15327752jpa8701_09
72. Abidin RR. Parenting Stress Index. 4th ed. FL: PAR (2012).
73. Rothbart MK, Ahadi SA, Hershey KL, Fisher P. Investigations of temperament at
three to seven years: the childrens behavior questionnaire. Child Dev. (2001) 72
(5):1394408. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00355
74. Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus (Version 8.10). Los Angeles, CA: Muthén &
Muthén (2023).
75. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders (Vol. 5, Issue 5). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association
(2022). doi: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425787
76. Ramirez CA, Rosén LA, Deffenbacher JL, Hurst H, Nicoletta C, Rosencranz T,
et al. Anger and anger expression in adults with high ADHD symptoms. J Atten
Disord. (1997) 2(2):11528. doi: 10.1177/108705479700200205
77. Braaten EB, Rosén LA. Self-regulation of affect in attention decit-hyperactivity
disorder (ADHD) and non-ADHD boys: differences in empathic responding.
J Consult Clin Psychol. (2000) 68(2):31321. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.68.2.313
78. Richards TL, Deffenbacher JL, Rosén LA. Driving anger and other driving-
related behaviors in high and low ADHD symptom coll ege students. J Atten Disord.
(2002) 6(1):2538. doi: 10.1177/108705470200600104
79. Konok V, Szőke R. Longitudinal associations of children
s hyperactivity/
inattention, peer relationship problems and mobile device use. Sustainability. (2022)
14(14):118. doi: 10.3390/su14148845
80. Zimmerman FJ, Christakis DA. Associations between content types of early
media exposure and subsequ ent attentional problems. Pediatrics. (2007) 120
(5):98692. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-3322
81. Nikkelen SWC, Valkenburg PM, Huizinga M, Bushman BJ. Media use and
ADHD-related behaviors in children and adolescents: a meta-analysis. Dev Psychol.
(2014) 50(9):222841. doi: 10.1037/a0037318
82. Kuntsi J, Pinto R, Price TS, van der Meere JJ, Frazier-Wood AC, Asherson P.
The separation of ADHD inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity symptoms:
pathways from genetic effects to cognitive impairments and symptoms. J Abnorm
Child Psychol. (2014) 42(1):12736. doi: 10.1007/s10802-013-9771-7
83. Toplak ME, Pitch A, Flora DB, Iwenofu L, Ghelani K, Jain U, et al. The unity and
diversity of inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity in ADHD: evidence for a general
factor with separable dimensions. J Abnorm Child Psychol. (2009) 37(8):113750.
doi: 10.1007/s10802-009-9336-y
84. Christakis DA, Benedikt Ramirez JS, Ferguson SM, Ravinder S, Ramirez JM.
How early media exposure may affect cognitive function: a review of results from
observations in humans and experiments in mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. (2018)
115(40):98518. doi: 10.1073/pnas.17 11548115
85. Flynn RM, Richert RA. Cognitive, not physical, engagement in video gaming
inuences executive functioning. J Cogn Dev. (2018) 19(1):120. doi: 10.1080/
15248372.2017.1419246
86. Huber B, Yeates M, Meyer D, Fleckhammer L, Kaufman J. The effects of screen
media content on young childrens executive functioning. J Exp Child Psychol. (2018)
170:7285. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2018.01.006
87. Jusienė R, Rakickienė L, Breidokienė R, Laurinaitytė I. Executive function and
screen-based media use in preschool children. Infant Child Dev. (2020) 29(1):e2173.
doi: 10.1002/icd.2173
88. Gross D, Conrad B, Fogg L, Wothke W. A longitudinal model of maternal self-
efcacy, depression, and difcult temperament during toddlerhood. Res Nurs Health.
(1994) 17(3):20715. doi: 10.1002/nur.4770170308
89. Takács L, Smo lík F, Putnam S. Assessing longitudinal pathways between
maternal depressive symptoms, parenting self-esteem and infant temperament. PLoS
One. (2019) 14(8):e0220633. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0220633
90. Fitzpatrick C, Binet MA, Harvey E, Barr R, Couture M, Garon-Carrier G.
Preschooler screen time and temperamental anger/frustration during the COVID-19
pandemic. Pediatr Res. (2023) 94(2):8205. doi: 10.1038/s41390-023-02485-6
91. Lim JY, Bae YJ. Validation study of Korean version of the Rothbarts childrens
behavior questionnaire.
Korean J Hum Ecol. (2015) 24(4):47797. doi: 10.5934/kjhe.
2015.24.4.477
92. Majdandžić M, Van Den Boom DC. Multimethod longitudinal assessment of
temperament in early childhood. J Pers. (2007) 75(1):12168. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-
6494.2006.00435.x
93. Majdandžić M, van den Boom DC, Heesbeen DG. Peas in a pod: biases in the
measurement of sibling temperament? Dev Psychol. (2008) 44(5 ):1354. doi: 10.1037/
a0013064
94. Ribner AD, Coulanges L, Friedman S, Libertus ME, Hughes C, Foley S, et al.
Screen time in the coronavirus 2019 era: international trends of increasing use
among 3- to 7-year-old children. J Pediatr. (2021) 239:5966.e1. doi: 10.1016/j.
jpeds.2021.08.068
95. Eales L, Gillespie S, Alstat RA, Ferguson GM, Carlson SM. Childrens screen and
problematic media use in the United States before and during the COVID-19
pandemic. Child Dev. (2021) 92(5):e86682. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13652
96. Sherer J, Levounis P. Technological addictions. Curr Psychiatry Rep. (2022) 24
(9):399406. doi: 10.1007/s11920-022-01351-2
Konok et al. 10.3389/frcha.2024.1276154
Frontiers in Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 12 frontiersin.org